Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Results of a pilot study on the involvement of bilateral inferior frontal gyri in emotional prosody perception: an rTMS study

Abstract

Background

The right hemisphere may play an important role in paralinguistic features such as the emotional melody in speech. The extent of this involvement however is unclear. Imaging studies have shown involvement of both left and right inferior frontal gyri in emotional prosody perception. The present pilot study examined whether these brain areas are critically involved in the processing of emotional prosody and of semantics in 9 healthy subjects. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation was used with a coil centred over left and right inferior frontal gyri, as localized by neuronavigation based on the subject's MRI. A sham condition was included. An online-TMS approach was applied; an emotional language task was completed during stimulation. This computerized task consisted of sentences pronounced by actors. In the semantics condition an emotion (fear, anger or neutral) was expressed in the content pronounced with a neutral intonation. In the prosody condition the emotion was expressed in the intonation, while the content was neutral.

Results

Reaction times on the emotional prosody task condition were significantly longer after rTMS over both the right and the left inferior frontal gyrus as compared to sham stimulation and after controlling for learning effects associated with order of condition. When taking all emotions together, there was no difference in effect on reaction times between the right and left stimulation. For the emotion Fear, reaction times were significantly longer after stimulating the left inferior frontal gyrus as compared to the right inferior frontal gyrus. Reaction times in the semantics task condition were not significantly different between the three TMS conditions.

Conclusions

The data indicate a critical involvement of both the right and the left inferior frontal gyrus in emotional prosody perception. The findings of this pilot study need replication. Future studies should include more subjects and examine whether the left and right inferior frontal gyrus play a differential role and complement each other, e.g. in the integrated processing of linguistic and prosodic aspects of speech, respectively.

Background

In auditory language processing, distinct brain areas serve different aspects of language. Language has been attributed to the left hemisphere since Broca (1861) and Wernicke (1874). Their studies showed that articulate speech and verbal comprehension are disrupted by left but not right hemisphere lesions [1]. Emotional prosody, a paralinguistic feature of language, is characterized by intonation, loudness and stress placement in speech. The emotional prosody of spoken language may convey crucial information about the emotional state of the speaker. Not only what is said but also how it is said gives significant information about the speaker's true communicative intent and is therefore crucial for proficient social interaction [2]. Studies examining the neural substrate of emotional prosody perception have revealed a network including bilateral regions in superior and middle temporal gyri and orbital and inferior frontal regions. Some studies have also implicated sub cortical structures such as the amygdala [3, 4] and the basal ganglia [5, 6]. Lesion and imaging studies have suggested that processing of affective prosodic information may be differentially lateralized when compared to linguistic, semantic processing. Whereas (in right-handers) the left hemisphere seems to be specialized for semantic and syntactic components of speech, the right hemisphere appears to be dominant in non-lexical components, such as affective prosody and gestural signs in communication [7, 8]. Lesion and imaging studies show, however, discrepant data to the extent of "right lateralization" of emotional prosody processing. A study that directly compared emotional prosody discrimination against discrimination of emotional semantics revealed right lateralized activity during detection of emotional prosody and left lateralized activity as a response to emotional semantics [9]. A number of imaging studies have also shown right lateralized activity during the perception of emotional prosody [3, 9–14]. Other imaging studies on emotional prosody perception have however shown neural responses in both right and left hemispheres [15–22]. Deficits in emotional prosody perception have also been found after left hemisphere lesions [23, 24]. To further define the exact neural substrate of emotional prosody perception and the extent of its lateralization, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), a brain mapping technique that allows causal inferences between neural activity and performance at a behavioural level [25, 26]. A recent TMS study provided evidence of right-hemisphere involvement in emotional prosody discrimination [27]. Increased reaction times were observed after 12 min of 1 Hz TMS (90% of the motor threshold) over the right fronto-parietal operculum relative to a left-hemisphere sham condition. This effect was specific for emotional prosody discrimination and was not found for discriminating emotional semantics. Detection of withdrawal emotions and not of approach emotions in prosody was delayed significantly by TMS, in accordance with accounts of the neural implementation of approach and withdrawal systems [28].

The region of interest, the right fronto-parietal operculum, in the study of van Rijn et al., was based on a lesion study from Adolphs et al. [23, 27]. The present experiment was a sequel and extension to the study of van Rijn [27]. The target regions of our study were not based on lesion studies, however, but on imaging studies revealing an association with bilateral inferior frontal gyri [14, 21]. We used on-line TMS in order to test the following hypotheses. First, both left and right inferior frontal gyri are critically involved in emotional prosody perception. Second, emotional semantics and emotional prosody can be dissociated at a neuro-anatomical level. Finally, there is a difference in lateralization between withdrawal (fear) and approach (anger) emotions.

Methods

Participants

Ten subjects (6 females, 4 males), students from the University of Groningen aged between 18 and 26 years (mean 21.8, s.d. 2.6 years) participated in the study. Right-handedness was confirmed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory by using the scoring method: (Total left - total right)/(total left + total right) * 100. Results < -40 = left-handed, between -40 and + 40 = ambidextruous and results > + 40 = right-handed. They had at least a high school level of understanding of Dutch (comprehension and reading). Participants were screened for TMS exclusion criteria [29] and MRI exclusion criteria. None had a (family) history of psychiatric or neurological problems or implants. They were given extensive written and oral explanation of the procedures and signed an informed consent. The experiment was conducted in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki and local ethics committee approval (University Medical Centre Groningen).

Experimental setup

Structural scanning was done on a Philips Intera 3 T MR-system at the BCN Neuroimaging Center, Groningen. The regions of interest were drawn in MRIcro a few days before the TMS experiment by the primary investigator and checked by another researcher until the two reached consensus, see Figure 1. Number of voxels per region of interest was ± 120, 1 to 2 cm, conform the size of the region TMS affects [30]. The bilateral inferior frontal gyrus was defined as BA 45/46 conform Ethofer et.al. [21]. For the TMS procedure, subjects were seated in a comfortable chair, in front of a computer screen and a keyboard with coloured response keys. A neural navigator (NeNa) was used to reliably localize the desired stimulation areas, of the participant [31]. This frameless stereotaxy device allows using a subject's structural MRI scan to navigate a TMS coil to the proper location on the skull. At the beginning of each TMS session the left and right inferior frontal gyri were marked on a tightly fit rubber head cap. For TMS, we used a MagStim Rapid magnetic stimulator (MagStim Co, Whitland, UK) with a figure of eight coil with a diameter of 70 mm for each loop. Then, individual motor thresholds of the left hemisphere were determined using the thumb (abductor pollicis brevis) movement procedure [32]. Stimulation intensity was set at 90% of the motor threshold, mean stimulation intensity was 52.1% (s.d. 3.1). In one subject the stimulation intensity had to be decreased from 59 to 54 because of an uncomfortable feeling while stimulating the right inferior frontal gyrus. In another subject we could not succeed in determining the motor threshold, for this subject the stimulation intensity was set at 50 of maximum output. Maximum output of the TMS machine is 2.5 Tesla. The coil was tightly maintained in a constant position with a special developed arm. The orientation of the coil was set with the handle pointing downwards, making an angle of 90° with the midline of the head.

Figure 1
figure 1

Region of interest, the inferior frontal gyrus, drawn in MRIcro.

Emotional language task

The emotional language task consisted of two conditions, prosody and semantics. These were matched in design. In the affective prosody condition subjects had to attend to the intonation of the voice and ignore the neutral content. The sentences of neutral content are pronounced in an emotional (anxious, angry) or neutral tone of voice by two professional actors, a male and a female voice, to control for individual and/or gender differences in affective prosody. All sentences were Dutch. Examples of the sentences with neutral content are, "The old car drives through the streets of the capital" and "Jan has been going to the hairdresser". In the semantics condition, subjects had to attend to the emotional (anxious, angry) or neutral content, and ignore the neutral intonation. Examples of sentences with an emotional content are, "Desperately he threw the glass from the table" and "The face stared with a pale face". The sentences were selected from two different validated prosody tests [33, 34]. The digitized stimuli for both conditions were all of approximately equal length (about seven words) and presented through two computer sound boxes (duration varying from 1560 to 3050 msec). The task was developed and presented using Eprime software [35]. During listening, the emotions to be discriminated, anger, fear or neutral, were presented on the computer screen. The visual presentation of the answer choices was included to aid subjects as to which categories they were to choose from. Without the visual presentation, subjects would have to memorize the different categories, and the task would have a stronger working memory component. As soon as participants identified the emotion expressed in the tone of voice, they were required to use the index finger of their right hand to make a "fear" response on a keypad, the middle finger to make an "angry" response or the thumb for a "neutral" response. Speed and accuracy were stressed.

TMS Procedure

At the beginning of each TMS session, subjects completed the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS), to assess their current affective state [36] (Dutch translation from Peeters et al., [37], validated by Boon and Peeters, [38]). Before the TMS stimulation started, participants performed a practice task for one of the two conditions (semantics or prosody) to get used to the task. An on-line task design was employed, i.e. participants completed the tasks during the TMS stimulation. The design used was described in a recent study on covert speech arrest induced by rTMS, done by Aziz-Zadeh et al. [39]. The experiment ran in three blocks of the same length, corresponding to the three TMS conditions: the two stimulated scalp positions and a sham condition; the areas of stimulation were the left and right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). A third condition concerned the sham control condition, which was over the right IFG. In the sham condition we used a placebo coil, of which the manifestation and the clicking sound are similar to a real coil. During (sham) stimulation of each area 48 stimuli, consisting of 24 trials of each task condition (semantics and prosody), resulting in 8 trials per emotion (anger, fear or neutral), were presented. Order of conditions and TMS stimulated scalp positions were counterbalanced across subjects. RTMS during the auditorally presented stimuli consisted of a train of 12 pulses at 5 Hz. The train was delivered starting 200 msec prior to stimulus presentation. A new stimulus was presented every 10 sec. The time interval between the TMS trains was 7600 msec. The TMS blocks were separated by at least 30 min in order to minimize the possibility of carry-over effects [40]. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible by pressing one of three coloured keys on a keypad. The entire procedure took 160 min at maximum in one session.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences 14.0.0 (2005). Accuracy, described as percentage correct, and reaction times for detection of emotion in semantics and prosody were chosen as dependent variables. As participants performed highly accurately (prosody condition 90% on average (s.d. 4.7), semantics condition 92% (s.d. 7.9)), only reaction times for correct trials were included in the analyses. In the design used in this study, every experimental trial was accompanied either by real TMS or sham stimulation. A 2 Ă— 3 Ă— 3 repeated measures ANOVA with task condition (prosody, semantics), TMS condition (left inferior frontal gyrus, right inferior frontal gyrus or sham) and emotion (fear, anger or neutral) as within subject factors and 'order of TMS conditions' as between subjects variable was used to test the effect of TMS on distinct location on the two tasks for the three emotions. Given that we used a within-subjects design in which subjects received different TMS (or sham) conditions in one session, we had to take into account learning effects as a result of repeated testing. Learning effects due to repeated testing have been shown in a variety of neuropsychological tests [41] and can be so large that they might obscure experimental effects. Therefore, the variable 'order of TMS conditions' was included as a between subjects factor in the repeated measures ANOVA, to control for the order of TMS conditions. One subject scored a percentage correct of 38% in the sham condition of the semantics task; we excluded her results from the analysis because this percentage is close to chance level. Results from nine subjects were included in the analysis. Results with p-values < .05 were regarded as significant.

Results

Mood and speed-accuracy trade off

One-factor repeated measures ANOVA, with TMS condition as a factor, revealed that there was no relation between the TMS conditions and the PANAS positive and negative scales. Therefore, it is unlikely that differences in reaction times and accuracy scores between the TMS conditions can be accounted for by changes in mood. There was no speed-accuracy trade off effect: no correlation was found between reaction times and accuracy scores. Results also did not reveal learning effects; accuracy scores and reaction times were equal per subject over the conditions. All effects were tested at a significance level of p < .05.

Accuracy

With regard to accuracy, no significant differences between the TMS conditions or any interaction effects were found. Mean percentage correct in the semantics condition was 92% (s.d. 7.9) and in the prosodic condition, 90% (s.d. 4.7).

Reaction times

For mean reaction times, a main effect of task was found, F (1, 8) = 19. 4, p < .05. Longer reaction times were found in the semantics condition than in the prosody condition, mean reaction times were 2412 msec (s.d. 629 msec) and 2063 msec (s.d. 443 msec) respectively. Therefore both tasks were analysed separately.

Analysis on the prosody task, with mean reaction times for the three TMS conditions (sham, left inferior frontal gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus) as within subjects' variable and order of TMS conditions as between subjects' variable, revealed a main effect of TMS condition, F (2,8) = 13.4, p < .01. If all other variables are ignored, reaction times were different between the three TMS conditions for the prosodic task condition. Contrasts revealed that reaction times were longer after rTMS over both left and right inferior frontal gyrus as compared to sham, F (1, 4) = 13.3, p < .05 and F (1, 4) = 15.7, p < .05 respectively. There was also no difference between TMS over left inferior frontal gyrus and TMS over right inferior frontal gyrus, F (1, 4) = 5.3, p = .08 when comparing reaction times on the prosody task. There was however, also an interaction between TMS condition and order of TMS conditions, F (8, 8) = 20.2, p < .001. This indicates that differences in reaction times between the TMS conditions in the prosodic task condition (left IFG, right IFG and sham) depend on the order of TMS conditions. Although this interaction was found, the results shown in Figure 2 and the main effect of TMS condition show that reaction times in the prosody condition were longer after both left and right TMS, after correction for order of TMS conditions. The estimated means show that reaction times after real TMS are longer, if the sham condition is included as second or third condition.

Figure 2
figure 2

Reaction times on prosody task for the three TMS conditions. Mean reaction times per TMS condition on the emotional prosody task corrected for order of TMS conditions. Separate lines represent the three emotions, fear, anger and neutral.

To test the third hypothesis: there is a difference in lateralization between withdrawal (fear) and approach (anger) emotions, Emotion was added as a within subjects' variable. No difference between the three emotions was found, p = .21 and no interaction between emotion and condition, when taking into account the order of TMS condition, p = .32. Therefore, the three emotions should be analysed together. When looking at the separate lines for the emotions in Figure 2, however, there seems to be a difference between the TMS conditions, for the emotion fear. When analyzing this emotion separately, there is indeed a significant difference between the three TMS conditions, after correcting for the order of TMS conditions, F (8, 8) = 3.48, p < .05. Contrasts revealed significant longer reaction times after stimulating the left inferior frontal gyrus than after stimulating the right inferior frontal gyrus in detecting the emotion fear from prosody, F (4, 4) = 9.48, p < .05). For the emotion anger, no effect of condition was found, p = 0.13. The difference represented in Figure 2 between the lines representing fear and anger is not significant, no interaction was found between TMS condition and emotion, p =.32. Although the relation looks quadratic, contrasts revealed no difference in reaction times between fear and anger depending on the TMS condition, p = .12.

Analysis on the reaction times in the semantics task condition, with TMS condition and Emotion as within subjects' variables and order of TMS conditions as between subjects' variable revealed no significant differences between TMS conditions and no interaction effects.

Discussion

This pilot study was designed to test three hypotheses: 1) Both left and right inferior frontal gyri are critically involved in emotional prosody perception. 2) Emotional semantics and emotional prosody can be dissociated at a neuro-anatomical level. 3) There is a difference in lateralization between withdrawal (fear) and approach (anger) emotions. The first hypothesis was supported by the results of this pilot study. Participants were slower in correctly classifying emotion from prosody during TMS over both left and right inferior frontal gyrus as compared to a sham condition. This indicates that both the left and the right inferior frontal gyrus are crucially involved in emotional prosody perception. Our findings thus support a recent theoretical framework, proposed by Schirmer and Kotz [42]. These authors argue that both left and right are involved in the processing of emotional prosody, albeit with different roles. More specifically, the right inferior frontal gyrus would be involved in evaluative judgments, while the left inferior frontal gyrus would sub serve the integration with other co-occurring processes [42]. The target regions of the present study were based on an imaging study of Ethofer et al. [21]. This study has lent support for a cooperation of the left and right inferior frontal gyrus in affective prosody perception, by testing their effective connectivity [21]. These authors performed a connectivity analysis which indicated a flow of information along parallel projections from the right posterior superior temporal cortex to the bilateral inferior frontal cortices [21]. In our study stimuli in the prosody condition consisted of sentences with a neutral semantic content. Both evaluative judgment of the prosodic intonation and integration with co-occurring processes, like the integration with emotional semantic meaning from earlier experiences, were needed to be able to choose the correct emotion. Inhibiting one of these two areas is enough to deteriorate reaction times on this task. Apparently, imaging studies have already shown the involvement of both left and right frontal areas in emotional prosody perception. As far as we know, this is the first study with TMS showing the crucial involvement of left and right inferior frontal gyrus in emotional prosody perception, which gives a stronger test of causal involvement. This finding leaves open a possible rightward asymmetrical activation at a temporal level, as has been found in imaging studies [9, 11, 12, 21]. No effects were found on accuracy measures, this is as expected, because virtual lesions induced by TMS generally manifest in reaction times rather than in percentages correct [43]. Our data did not show significant involvement of the left inferior frontal gyrus in emotional semantics. This is in contradiction with other studies examining the neuro-anatomical substrate of processing emotional information from semantics [9, 27, 44]. This might be explained by the high inter-subject variability and the sensitivity of the semantics condition. It could have been that the sentences used as stimuli and the number of emotions (the two emotions anger and fear and neutral) in the semantics condition was too easy. Furthermore, the absence of effect in the semantic condition can be due to lack of power. The second hypothesis of a differential involvement of the right hemisphere in emotional prosody and of the left hemisphere in emotional semantics that was found in an imaging study [9] was not supported by our data. Our study could not dissociate the processing of emotional semantics and emotional prosody at a neuro-anatomical level. The third hypothesis on a difference in lateralization between withdrawal (fear) and approach (anger) emotions could also not be supported by our data. Performances on both emotions in the prosody condition deteriorated after rTMS over left and over right inferior frontal gyrus. Interestingly, the separate analysis of reaction times for the detection of the emotion fear from prosody revealed significantly longer reaction times after TMS over the left inferior frontal gyrus as compared to stimulation over the right inferior frontal gyrus. This finding is in contradiction with accounts of the neural implementation of approach and withdrawal systems [28] and with the findings from a recent TMS study of van Rijn et al. [27]. In that study however, a different brain area was targeted (fronto-parietal operculum) and that study lacked a left TMS condition. Furthermore, more emotions were included, not only anger and fear, as in our study, but also happiness and sadness. This could have made the task more sensitive and did result in more trials after splitting in withdrawal and approach emotions. Some limitations of present study should be noted. The first concern is the use of a placebo coil. Although its manifestation and the clicking sound are similar to a real coil, it does not give the same sensations as real TMS. That is, sensations are absent for the placebo coil. We cannot exclude general TMS induced effects on attention as a consequence of the sensations. Other sham conditions however, also have drawbacks. It has been shown, that tilting the coil by 45 to 90 degrees, which is the most frequently used sham condition, might still affect brain activity [45]. Another option is to include an active control condition, a region that has never been related to the function of interest. This method can also not be regarded as an unaffected baseline, because it may cause the general TMS induced effects on attention. Inclusion of temporal cortex regions in the design could give a stronger test of lateralization of emotional information processing in both [21] semantics and in prosody.

Conclusions

In summary, our pilot data lend evidence from TMS for a crucial involvement of both right and left inferior frontal gyrus in emotional prosody perception consistent with earlier fMRI findings [21]. The findings of this pilot study need replication. Future research should investigate whether the left and right inferior frontal gyrus play a differential role and complement each other, e.g. in the integrated processing of linguistic and prosodic aspects of speech, respectively [42]. The number of subjects should be higher in future studies.

References

  1. Ross ED, Thompson RD, Yenkosky J: Lateralization of affective prosody in brain and the callosal integration of hemispheric language functions. Brain and language. 1997, 56: 27-54. 10.1006/brln.1997.1731.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Mitchell RL, Crow TJ: Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: the forgotten hemisphere?. Brain. 2005, 128: 963-978. 10.1093/brain/awh466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Phillips ML, Young AW, Scott SK, Calder AJ, Andrew C, Giampietro V, Williams SC, Bullmore ET, Brammer M, Gray JA: Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 1998, 265: 1809-1817. 10.1098/rspb.1998.0506.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Sander D, Grandjean D, Pourtois G, Schwartz S, Seghier ML, Scherer KR, Vuilleumier P: Emotion and attention interactions in social cognition: Brain regions involved in processing anger prosody. NeuroImage. 2005, 28: 848-858. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Pell MD, Leonard CL: Processing emotional tone from speech in Parkinson's disease: a role for the basal ganglia. Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience. 2003, 3: 275-288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Cancelliere AE, Kertesz A: Lesion localization in acquired deficits of emotional expression and comprehension. Brain and cognition. 1990, 13: 133-147. 10.1016/0278-2626(90)90046-Q.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Ross ED, Orbelo DM, Cartwright J, Hansel S, Burgard M, Testa JA, Buck R: Affective-prosodic deficits in schizophrenia: profiles of patients with brain damage and comparison with relation to schizophrenic symptoms. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2001, 70: 597-604. 10.1136/jnnp.70.5.597.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ross ED: The aprosodias. Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. 2003, McGraw Hill. New York

    Google Scholar 

  9. Mitchell RL, Elliott R, Barry M, Cruttenden A, Woodruff PW: The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41: 1410-1421. 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00017-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. George MS, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, Ketter TA, Kimbrell TA, Heilman KM, Herscovitch P, Post RM: Understanding emotional prosody activates right hemisphere regions. Archives of neurology. 1996, 53: 665-670.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Wildgruber D, Riecker A, Hertrich I, Erb M, Grodd W, Ethofer T, Ackermann H: Identification of emotional intonation evaluated by fMRI. NeuroImage. 2005, 24: 1233-1241. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.034.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Beaucousin V, Lacheret A, Turbelin M, Morel M, Mazoyer B, Tzourio-Mazoyer N: FMRI study of emotional speech comprehension. Cerebral cortex. 2007, 17: 339-352. 10.1093/cercor/bhj151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Imaizumi S, Mori K, Kiritani S, Kawashima R, Sugiura M, Fukuda H, Itoh K, Kato T, Nakamura A, Hatano K, et al.: Vocal identification of speaker and emotion activates different brain regions. Neuroreport. 1997, 8: 2809-2812. 10.1097/00001756-199708180-00031.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Buchanan TW, Lutz K, Mirzazade S, Specht K, Shah NJ, Zilles K, Jancke L: Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research. 2000, 9: 227-238. 10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00060-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Morris JS, Scott SK, Dolan RJ: Saying it with feeling: neural responses to emotional vocalizations. Neuropsychologia. 1999, 37: 1155-1163. 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00015-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Johnstone T, van Reekum CM, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ: The voice of emotion: an FMRI study of neural responses to angry and happy vocal expressions. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006, 1: 242-249. 10.1093/scan/nsl027.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Wildgruber D, Pihan H, Ackermann H, Erb M, Grodd W: Dynamic brain activation during processing of emotional intonation: influence of acoustic parameters, emotional valence, and sex. NeuroImage. 2002, 15: 856-869. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0998.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Kotz SA, Meyer M, Alter K, Besson M, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD: On the lateralization of emotional prosody: An event-related functional MR investigation. Brain and language. 2003, 86: 366-376. 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00532-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Meyer M, Alter K, Friederici AD, Lohmann G, von Cramon DY: FMRI reveals brain regions mediating slow prosodic modulations in spoken sentences. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002, 17: 73-88. 10.1002/hbm.10042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ethofer T, Kreifelts B, Wiethoff S, Wolf J, Grodd W, Vuilleumier P, Wildgruber D: Differential Influences of Emotion, Task, and Novelty on Brain Regions Underlying the Processing of Speech Melody. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2008, 21 (7): 1255-68. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Ethofer T, Anders S, Erb M, Herbert C, Wiethoff S, Kissler J, Grodd W, Wildgruber D: Cerebral pathways in processing of affective prosody: A dynamic causal modeling study. NeuroImage. 2006, 30: 580-587. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.059.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Wildgruber D, Hertrich I, Riecker A, Erb M, Anders S, Grodd W, Ackermann H: Distinct Frontal Regions Subserve Evaluation of Linguistic and Emotional Aspects of Speech Intonation. Cerebral cortex. 2004, 14: 1384-1389. 10.1093/cercor/bhh099.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D: Neural systems for recognition of emotional prosody: a 3-D lesion study. Emotion. 2002, 2: 23-51. 10.1037/1528-3542.2.1.23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Hornak J, Bramham J, Rolls ET, Morris RG, O'Doherty J, Bullock PR, Polkey CE: Changes in emotion after circumscribed surgical lesions of the orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Brain. 2003, 126: 1691-1712. 10.1093/brain/awg168.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V, Rothwell JC: Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience--virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2000, 10: 232-237. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00081-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kucharska-Pietura K, Phillips ML, Gernand W, David AS: Perception of emotions from faces and voices following unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41: 1082-1090. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00294-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. van Rijn S, Aleman A, van Diessen E, Berckmoes C, Vingerhoets G, Kahn RS: What is said or how it is said makes a difference: role of the right fronto-parietal operculum in emotional prosody as revealed by repetitive TMS. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005, 21: 3195-3200. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04130.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. d'Alfonso AAL, van Honk J, Hermans E, Postma A, de Haan EHF: Laterality effects in selective attention to threat after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at the prefrontal cortex in female subjects. Neuroscience Letters. 2000, 280: 195-198. 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00781-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wassermann EM: Risk and Safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and siggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation, June 5-7,1996. 1998

    Google Scholar 

  30. Walsh V, Cowey A: Transcranial magnetic stimulation and cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2000, 1: 73-79. 10.1038/35036239.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Neggers SFW, Langerak TR, Schutter DJLG, Mandl RCW, Ramsey NF, Lemmens PJJ, Postma A: A stereotactic method for image-guided transcranial magnetic stimulation validated with fMRI and motor-evoked potentials. NeuroImage. 2004, 21: 1805-1817. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Pridmore S, Fernandes FJA, Nahas Z, Liberatos C, George MS: Motor threshold in transcranial magnetic stimulation: A comparison of a neurophysiological method and a visualization of movement method. The journal of ECT. 1998, 14: 25-10.1097/00124509-199803000-00004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Vroomen J, Collier R, Mozziconacci S: Duration and intonation in emotional speech. 577-580.

  34. Vingerhoets G, Berckmoes C, Stroobant N: Cerebral hemodynamics during discrimination of prosodic and semantic emotion in speech studied by transcranial Doppler ultrasonography. Neuropsychology. 2003, 17: 93-99. 10.1037/0894-4105.17.1.93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A: E-Prime User's Guide. 2002, Pittsburgh, Psychology Software Tools Inc

    Google Scholar 

  36. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A: Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988, 54: 1063-1070. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Peeters F, Ponds R, Vermeeren M: Affectiviteit en zelfbeoordeling van depressie en angst. Tijdschrift voor psychiatrie. 1996, 38: 240-250.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Boon MTG, Peeters FPML: Affectieve dimensies bij depressie en angst. Tijdschrift voor psychiatrie. 1999, 41: 109-113.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Aziz-Zadeh L, Cattaneo L, Rochat M, Rizzolatti G: Covert speech arrest induced by rTMS over both motor and nonmotor left hemisphere frontal sites. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2005, 17: 928-938. 10.1162/0898929054021157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Kosslyn SM, Pascual-Leone A, Felician O, Camposano S, Keenan JP, Thompson WL, Ganis G, Sukel KE, Alpert NM: The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science. 1999, 284: 167-170. 10.1126/science.284.5411.167.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Wilson BA, Watson PC, Baddeley AD, Emslie H, Evans JJ: Improvement or simply practice? The effects of twenty repeated assessments on people with and without brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2000, 6: 469-479. 10.1017/S1355617700644053.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Schirmer A, Kotz S: Beyond the right hemisphere: brain mechanisms mediating vocal emotional processing. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2006, 10: 24-30. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Wassermann E, Epstein CM, Ziemann U: Oxford handbook of transcranial stimulation. 2008, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  44. Baum SR, Pell MD: The neural bases of prosody: Insights from lesion studies and neuroimaging. Aphasiology. 1999, 13: 581-608. 10.1080/026870399401984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Loo C, Taylor J, Gandevia S, McDarmont B, Mitchell P, Sachdev P: Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in controlled treatment studies: are some "sham" forms active?. Biological psychiatry. 2000, 47: 325-331. 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00285-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Prof. J. Vroomen, for allowing us to make use of his stimuli. The authors also thank the participants of this study. Furthermore, we are grateful to dr. R. Renken who helped us with the experimental setup.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marjolijn Hoekert.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

Authors MH and AA designed the study, MH wrote the article and did the statistical analyses, both thoroughly discussed with AA. GV helped with the interpretation of the data, checked the manuscript for completeness, and gave good suggestions for improvement of the text. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hoekert, M., Vingerhoets, G. & Aleman, A. Results of a pilot study on the involvement of bilateral inferior frontal gyri in emotional prosody perception: an rTMS study. BMC Neurosci 11, 93 (2010). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2202-11-93

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2202-11-93

Keywords