Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

The anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex are associated with avoidance of dental treatment based on prior experience of treatment in healthy adults

Abstract

Background

Fear concerning stressful medical or dental procedures is one of the major factors that distance patients from health care. Fear and avoidance of dental treatments can be shaped by a patient’s prior experience with receiving dental procedures or by imagining the procedures.

Methods

We performed two functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments to investigate the role of the anterior insula (aINS) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), which are both critical to threat perception, in dental avoidance. Dental avoidance based on both prior treatment experience and imagination was assessed using a customized questionnaire. In an fMRI task-based study, we investigated brain activation in 17 healthy participants when they viewed images depicting dental procedures that evoked a moderate degree of fear. Region-of-interest analysis was performed to assess the association between dental avoidance and aINS as well as dACC activation. In a resting state fMRI study, we investigated 18 healthy participants for the association between the intrinsic functional connectivity of the aINS and dACC and dental avoidance.

Results

We found that (1) the participants showed a higher activation of the right aINS and bilateral dACC when they viewed images of dental procedures compared with the brain activation observed when they viewed scrambled images (p < 0.05 corrected for small volume and family-wise error). (2) The avoidance ratings based on prior experience of dental treatment were significantly positively correlated with the activation in the right aINS (r = 0.67, p = 0.003), right dACC (r = 0.65, p = 0.005) and left dACC (r = 0.63, p = 0.007). (3) The intrinsic functional connectivity between the aINS and the orbitofrontal cortex was positively correlated with the avoidance ratings based on experience (uncorrected p < 0.001).

Conclusions

The findings highlight prior experience of dental treatment as a predominant factor in shaping patients’ avoidance behavior. Individual differences in threat perception may play a key role in the development of dental avoidance.

Background

Fear concerning stressful medical or dental procedures, such as gastrointestinal endoscopy, bronchoscopy or wisdom tooth extraction, is one of the major factors that distances patients from health care [1–4]. The fear of dental treatment is strongly associated with avoidance of dental treatment [5, 6], which results in deteriorated oral health, potentiates the further fear of treatment, and reduces a patient’s willingness to receive treatment [2, 6]. Fear and avoidance can be derived from one’s experience of prior dental treatment, such as a prior traumatic treatment experience being associated with increased fear toward treatment [7, 8], or it can be due to an imagined fear that is typically shaped by verbal instruction (e.g., messages from the media) or observation [9]. However, it remains unclear how the experience of prior dental treatment influences avoidance because the underlying neural mechanisms have not been fully investigated.

Cumulating evidence has revealed that anterior insula (aINS) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) activation is associated with fear and avoidance. First, research on dental phobia has revealed that visual (e.g., images depicting dental procedures), auditory (e.g., drilling sounds) or cross-modal materials (e.g., video of dentistry actions) [10–14] evoke fearful experiences and heightened activation at both the dACC and aINS [10, 13], together with the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [10, 12, 13]. Second, aINS and dACC activation has been associated with painful experiences when the subjects imagined [15–17] or recalled a painful experience [18–20], even without actually receiving the stimuli. Third, research on fear-defensive behavior has revealed that increased dental fear is associated with heightened defensive reactivity in the presentation of a painful threat [21]. Both the dACC and aINS are critical components in the defensive fear system [22], and activation was found in both the aINS and dACC when subjects anticipated or encountered a threatening object [23, 24]. The convergent findings suggest that the aINS and dACC may play a key role in dental fear and avoidance. Finally, long-term experience is able to shape brain function and structure, an effect known as neuroplasticity [25], and altered brain connectivity of the aINS and the dACC is associated with long-term psychological stress [26, 27]. Therefore, aINS and dACC connectivity may be associated with the degree of dental avoidance, which could be associated with a prior stressful experience [7, 28, 29].

The current study aimed to investigate the neural correlates of dental avoidance by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Two fMRI studies were performed to test the following hypotheses regarding the neural correlates underlying dental avoidance:

  1. 1.

    In the task-based fMRI study, we used visual stimuli to evoke fear about dental treatment. We hypothesized that brain activation of the aINS and the dACC would be positively correlated with the degree of dental avoidance.

  2. 2.

    In the resting-state fMRI study, we investigated whether there was an association between the intrinsic functional connectivity and the individual differences in dental avoidance. We hypothesized that aINS and dACC connectivity would be positively correlated with the degree of dental avoidance.

Methods

Participants

Twenty-one healthy participants were recruited from a university campus via bulletin board notices. All participants were screened for the following exclusion criteria: (1) having a history of major physical or psychiatric disorders including epilepsy, major depression, schizophrenia or neurovascular diseases, (2) having a history of brain injury or having undergone brain surgery, and (3) being unable to undergo MRI due to physical (e.g., having a surgical implant) or psychological (e.g., claustrophobia) contraindications. Seventeen participants (eight females, age 24.2 ± 2.0 (mean ± standard deviation)) participated in the ‘dental threat’ fMRI experiment. Eighteen participants (nine females, age 24.5 ± 2.1) participated in the ‘resting-state’ fMRI experiment. Fourteen of the 21 participants participated in both experiments. None of the participants had a history of major physical or psychiatric disorders or a history of chronic orofacial pain as assessed by a dentist (the first author, C-S L). Notably, because the current study aimed to investigate the neural correlates associated with the degree of dental avoidance, we did not exclusively recruit the participants with extreme dental fear, as done in previous studies [10, 12]. Therefore, the participants here represented a group of both low dental anxiety and high dental anxiety patients (see Results). All participants provided written informed consent before participating in this study. The study conforms to the ethical standards presented in the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Taipei Veterans General Hospital (VGHIRB No. 2013-06-026BY).

The number of participants was determined based a power analysis on the behavioral findings. Because we focused on the strength of association between the regional brain activation and the behavioral score (i.e., dental avoidance), we performed a power analysis to evaluate the sample size for a one-tailed bivariate correlation analysis with alpha = 0.05, beta = 0.8, and effect size (r) = 0.6. The effect size was estimated according to the previous findings [12], which showed the fear experience evoked by video was positively correlated with activation of the insula, the thalamus and the anterior cingulate, with r between 0.58 and 0.69. The analysis was performed using G*Power v3.1.9.2 [30] and showed a minimum of N = 15.

Experimental paradigm

The study consisted of two fMRI experiments. Seventeen participants completed a task-based ‘dental threat’ experiment. Dental avoidance based on an experience of prior treatment (i.e., experience-based avoidance) and imagination (i.e., imagination-based avoidance) was assessed using a customized questionnaire (Table 1). Eighteen participants completed a resting-state experiment.

Table 1 Customized questionnaire for dental avoidance

The ‘dental threat’ experiment

In the ‘dental threat’ study, we used visual images to elicit fear toward dental treatment. Such a paradigm has been adopted in previous studies concerning dental phobia [11, 12]. During the fMRI scan, 17 participants viewed a set of dental procedure images and performed the following tasks:

  1. (a)

    In the ‘Re-experiencing’ task (REXP), the participants were instructed to imagine that they were receiving the dental procedure and to imagine the pain or fear that they would experience from it. They were instructed to ‘Imagine that you are the subject who is receiving this treatment, and imagine the pain or fear that you would perceive, related to this treatment.’ The participants were asked to take a self-perspective as if they were in the same situation as depicted by the image [18] and to immerse themselves with these unpleasant feelings.

  2. (b)

    In the ‘counting’ task (COUNT), the participants were instructed to count how many teeth they could find in the image. They were instructed to ‘Count how many teeth that you find in this image.’

  3. (c)

    In the ‘scrambled image’ task (SCM), the participants were instructed to view a set of images displayed in the counting task that were scrambled as a visual control.

The whole experiment consisted of 20 REXP trials, 20 COUNT trials and 10 SCM trials, for a total of 50 trials (Fig. 1). Each REXP or COUNT trial consisted of three phases. In the initial cue phase, the word ‘imagine’ or ‘observe’ was displayed for 2 s to instruct the participants to perform the REXP or COUNT task, respectively, in the subsequent phase. In the stimulus phase, the image depicting the dental procedures was displayed for 9 s, and the participants performed a REXP or COUNT task according to the instruction given in the preceding cue phase. Finally, in the fixation phase, the participants were instructed to look at a fixed cross that was displayed for 3 or 5 s.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental paradigm. During the fMRI scan, the participants were asked to perform re-experience tasks (REXP, 20 trials), counting tasks (COUNT, 20 trials) and visual control tasks (SCM, 10 trials). The REXP and the COUNT tasks consisted of three phase: (1) cue (2 s), (2) stimulus (9 s for the REXP and the COUNT tasks and 6 s for the SCM task), and (3) fixation (3 or 5 s)

The ‘resting-state’ experiment

During the fMRI scan, 18 participants were instructed to be relaxed and remain awake with eyes open. They were instructed to fix their eyes on a cross symbol on the screen.

Preparation of images

Twenty images depicting 7 categories of common dental procedures including tooth extraction (N = 4), local anesthesia injection (N = 3), having a tooth surface polished (N = 4), drilling a tooth (N = 2), probing the gum (one image for the anterior site and one for the posterior site) (N = 2), exploring an anterior tooth (N = 4) and scaling an anterior tooth (N = 1) were created by photographing a healthy female subject who was not a study participant. All of the images were matched for brightness and hue using Adobe Photoshop® 7.0 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, California). The scrambled images were generated with a scramble filter tool (http://www.telegraphics.com.au/sw/). It should be noted that in this study, we aimed to elicit increased perceived threat by asking the participants to recall their past experiences of dental treatment rather than to directly evoke a strong aversive experience, such as disgust. Therefore, we photographed a subject with relatively good oral hygiene to minimize the potential feeling of disgust.

Assessment of behavioral data

Dental avoidance and trait dental anxiety

Dental avoidance was assessed using a customized self-report questionnaire that consisted of 12 dental procedure scenarios (Table 1). The dental procedures and descriptions were selected based on the fear of dental pain questionnaire (FDP) [31]. The participants were asked to indicate the degree of avoidance to each item using the previously described method [8]. For each participant, we distinguished the avoidance based on his/her past treatment experience from the avoidance based on imagination. For each scenario, the participants were required to (1) mark if they had received the procedure or not and (2) rate the degree to which they would avoid receiving the procedure. For each scenario, the participants were instructed to rate their avoidance based on their past experience about the procedure if they had received the procedure previously, or to rate avoidance based on their imagination if they had not experienced that procedure. The mean avoidance rating from the experienced procedures was indexed as the experience-based avoidance, and the mean avoidance rating from the non-experienced procedures was indexed as the imagination-based avoidance. All ratings were based on an 11-point numerical rating scale (NRS, 0 = the least degree and 10 = the maximal degree). Trait dental anxiety was assessed using the modified dental anxiety scale (MDAS) [32]. The Chinese version of the MDAS was used in the current study [33]. Its psychometric properties have been reported in a previous study, based on a Chinese-speaking sample [33]. The MDAS assessment was performed before the MRI scan. Statistical analysis was performed using Minitab version 16.1.0 (Minitab, Inc., State College, Pennsylvania).

Fear elicited by dental procedures

After the fMRI scan, the participants immediately rated how threatened they felt with respect to the images that they observed during the fMRI scan based on an 11-point numerical rating scale (NRS, 0 = not threatening at all and 10 = extremely threatening). The rating was indexed as the elicited fear. The participants were asked to briefly describe the dental procedures depicted by each image.

Acquisition and pre-processing of imaging data

The ‘dental threat’ experiment

The data were acquired on a 3-Tesla imaging system (Tim Trio, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a quadrature head coil. Functional data were acquired with T2-weighted gradient-echo EPI using blood-oxygenation-level-dependent contrast (TR/TE/flip angle = 2000 ms/20 ms/90°) with the following parameters: matrix size = 64 × 64 × 40 and a voxel size = 3.4 × 3.4 × 3.4 mm3. The total scan time was 6.5 min. An anatomical image was acquired using a T1-weighted 3D gradient-echo pulse sequence (modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform: TR/TE/TI = 2530/3.03/1100 ms) with the following parameters: matrix size = 256 × 256 × 192 and a voxel size = 1×1 × 1 mm3.

Functional imaging data were pre-processed and analyzed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The following protocol for pre-processing was based on our published study [34]. Scans were slice-time corrected, realigned and co-registered to the individual T1-weighted anatomical image before being normalized to a 2 × 2 × 2 mm MNI152 (Montreal Neurological Institute) space. Scans were further smoothed using a FWHM (full width at half maximum) 8 × 8 × 8 mm Gaussian kernel, high-pass filtered, and corrected for temporal serial correlations.

The ‘resting-state’ experiment

The data were acquired on the same scanner with the following parameters: [TR]/[TE]/flip angle = 2500 ms/30 ms/90°; matrix size = 64 × 64 × 40; voxel size = 3.4 × 3.4 × 3.4 mm3. The total scan time was 8.3 min. Pre-processing was performed using the Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF, http://www.restfmri.net) and the Resting-State fMRI Data Analysis Toolkit (REST, http://www.restfmri.net) based on the SPM8 software. The following pre-processing protocol was based on our published study [35]. All scans were slice-time corrected, corrected for head movement and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template. The time series from the seed region was band-pass filtered (0.01–0.1 Hz) to extract the low-frequency oscillating components that contributed to resting-state functional connectivity. The six movement parameters of rigid body translation and rotation and the mean signal of the cerebrospinal ventricles and the deep white matter were removed as nuisance regressors using multiple regression. Global signal regression was performed to reduce the influence of noise introduced by physiological activities.

Analysis of imaging data

The ‘dental threat’ experiment

At the individual level, we modeled the cue phase for 2 s, the fixation phase for 3 or 5 s, and the stimulus phases for 6 s (starting from the 4th second to the end of the phase) using a canonical hemodynamic response function. Head movement parameters were modeled as the regressors of no interest. A random-effect analysis was performed at the group level: a one-sample t test was performed for the first-level contrasts REXP > SCM and COUNT > SCM across all participants. An exploratory whole-brain analysis was performed by thresholding with an uncorrected p < 0.001 and a cluster size >10 voxels. Based on our primary hypothesis, we performed a region of interest (ROI)-based analysis with small volume correction (SVC) on the bilateral aINS and dACC ROIs. The masks of these ROIs were defined according to a previous study [36]. The results were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05 and were corrected for family-wise error (FWE). To quantify the strength of association between dental avoidance the aINS as well as dACC activation, we calculated the Pearson correlation coefficient between the regional activation of the aINS and the dACC ROIs and the ratings of experience- and imagination-based avoidance across all participants. Regional activation was quantified as the mean beta value from all of the voxels within the ROIs of the contrast REXP > SCM. The Pearson correlation coefficient was considered statistically significant at p < 0.05 (two-tailed test of significance).

The ‘resting-state’ experiment

We performed a seed-based functional connectivity (SBFC) analysis based on published methods [37]. The analysis revealed the functional connectivity related to the seed and was quantified as the correlation of brain activity patterns between the seed region and other parts of the brain. The aINS and dACC were used as the seed regions. The masks of the seed regions were defined based on the results from the dental threat experiment. Regression analysis was performed using the ratings of experience- or imagination-based avoidance as the covariate to reveal the brain region in which connectivity with the seed region was significantly correlated with the experience- or imagination-based avoidance. For an exploratory purpose, all imaging results were thresholded at an uncorrected p < 0.001, with a cluster size >10 voxels.

Results

Demographic and behavioral data

The demographic and behavioral data are summarized in Table 2. The customized self-report questionnaire of dental avoidance was evaluated for internal consistency using Cronbach’s alpha and for test–retest reliability using intraclass consistency (ICC). The assessment showed both good internal consistency (Cronbach’s Alpha = 0.90) and a test–retest reliability of 0.91. The scores of experience-based avoidance, imagination-based avoidance, the modified dental anxiety scale (MDAS), and elicited fear all followed a normal distribution as assessed by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for normal distribution. Across all participants, the avoidance ratings based on imagination were slightly greater than ratings based on experience, but the difference was not statistically significant (paired t-test p > 0.05 for both experiments) (Table 2A, B). In the ‘dental threat’ experiment, the participants reported a moderate degree of elicited fear (mean ± standard deviation = 5.0 ± 1.7) (Table 2A). A post-experiment survey revealed that all the participants were able to recognize the dental procedures depicted in the images. The ratings of elicited fear were significantly positively correlated with the MDAS score (r = 0.71, p = 0.001) (Table 2C; Fig. 2a), which indicated that the visual stimuli successfully elicited fear with respect to receiving dental procedures. The MDAS score showed a moderate degree of trait dental anxiety (mean ± standard deviation = 13.6 ± 3.9, min/max = 9/20) among the participants (Table 2A). Seven participants showed low dental anxiety (MDAS score 5–11), 8 showed moderated dental anxiety (MDAS score 12–18) and 2 showed high dental anxiety (MDAS score 19–25) based on the published cut-off points [32, 38]. The findings suggested that both low- and high-anxiety persons were included. Importantly, elicited fear was significantly positively correlated with the ratings of experience-based avoidance (r = 0.55, p = 0.02) but not with ratings of imagination-based avoidance (r = 0.25, p = 0.38) (Table 2B; Fig. 2a), which suggests a close association between fear and prior experience of dental treatment.

Table 2 Demographic and behavioral data
Fig. 2
figure 2

a The ratings of elicited fear were significantly correlated with the MDAS score and avoidance ratings based on experience. No significant correlation was found between the ratings of elicited fear and the avoidance ratings based on imagination. b Results of the ROI-based analysis. The anterior insula (aINS) and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) were selected as the regions of interest (ROIs) (the upper panel). The REXP task showed an increased activation in the right aINS and the bilateral dACC (the middle panel). The COUNT task showed an increased activation in the bilateral dACC (the lower panel). A cluster was considered statistically significant by small volume-correction based on the pre-defined ROIs (p < 0.05 corrected for family-wise error). c The ratings of experience-based avoidance were significantly correlated with the activation in the right aINS, the right dACC and the left dACC during the re-experience task

Results of the dental threat experiment

The findings of the ROI-based analysis are shown in Fig. 2b. When the participants viewed the treatment-related images and re-experienced their past experience of treatment (i.e., the re-experiencing task, REXP) compared with viewing the visual control (i.e., the scrambled image task, SCM), we found an increased activation in the right anterior insula (aINS) ([x,y,z] = [38,20,0], size = 23 voxels, Z = 3.46, p FWE-corrected = 0.01), in the right dACC ([x,y,z] = [12, 22, 30], size = 7 voxels, Z = 3.82, p FWE-corrected = 0.004), and in the left dACC ([x,y,z] = [−6,22,20], size = 15 voxels, Z = 3.38, p FWE-corrected = 0.015). When the participants counted the number of teeth (i.e., the counting task, COUNT) compared with the visual control, there was an increased activation in the bilateral dACC (Fig. 2b). In the REXP task, the post hoc correlation analyses revealed that the rating of experience-based avoidance was significantly correlated with brain activation in the right aINS (r = 0.67, p = 0.003), right dACC (r = 0.65, p = 0.005) and left dACC (r = 0.63, p = 0.007) (Fig. 2c). In contrast, in the COUNT task, the correlation was not statistically significant. The findings confirmed our hypothesis that aINS and dACC activation is associated with the degree of dental avoidance. The data from the exploratory whole-brain analysis are shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Clusters of significant brain activation in the re-experience and the counting tasks

It should be noted that the association between experience-based dental avoidance and brain activation, as reported above, can be confounded by the patient’s age and MDAS score (Table 2A). We therefore performed multiple regression analyses by modeling the regional activation as the dependent variable and the experience-based dental avoidance, age and the MDAS score as the predictors. The analyses showed that the avoidance ratings were the only significant predictor of brain activation in the ROIs (right aINS: p = 0.017; right dACC: p = 0.029; left dACC: p = 0.046).

Results of the resting-state experiment

When the right aINS was used as a seed (Fig. 3a), we found that its connectivity with the right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) was positively correlated with the ratings of experience-based avoidance ([x, y, z] = [28, 30, −20], size = 33 voxels, Z = 3.77, p uncorrected < 0.001) (Fig. 3b). No significant correlation was found between the right aINS-OFC connectivity and the ratings of imagination-based avoidance. The findings suggested that the connectional changes of the threat-related region (i.e., aINS) reflected the degree of dental avoidance that was shaped by prior experience of dental treatment. No significant finding was found when the dACC was used as a seed.

Fig. 3
figure 3

a The right anterior insula (aINS) was used as the seed region for the seed-based functional connectivity analysis. The region was defined as a sphere (diameter = 6 mm) centered at [x, y, z] = [38, 20, 0]. b The whole-brain exploratory analysis revealed that the connectivity between the right aINS and the right orbitofrontal cortex was significantly correlated with the ratings of recalled avoidance

Discussion

Summary of the major findings

Dental avoidance is closely associated with the fear of dental treatment. It can be shaped by a patient’s past experience with respect to receiving dental procedures or by imagination that is primarily derived from verbal or observational learning. Using two fMRI studies, we have demonstrated the following neural mechanisms underlying dental avoidance:

  1. 1.

    When fear was evoked by viewing images of dental procedures, the participants showed greater activation of the right aINS and bilateral dACC (Fig. 2b).

  2. 2.

    The aINS and dACC activation was positively correlated with dental avoidance based on experience but not dental avoidance based on imagination (Fig. 2c).

  3. 3.

    In the resting state, the intrinsic functional connectivity between the aINS and OFC was positively correlated with dental avoidance based on experience but not with dental avoidance based on imagination (Fig. 3c).

In sum, these findings revealed that the activation of the aINS and dACC, which are both critical to threat perception, is associated with the ratings of dental avoidance.

Dental avoidance and activation of the aINS and the dACC

In the ‘dental threat’ study, we found an increased activation in the aINS and dACC during the re-experience task, which evoked a moderate degree of fear (Fig. 2b). The significant correlation between the experience-based dental avoidance and the aINS as well as dACC activation (Fig. 2c) may reflect a heightened perceived threat when the participants viewed the treatment-related images. Both the aINS and dACC are critical to integrating salience signals (e.g., a painful procedure) from a threatening context, and the activation of these regions is associated with the individual difference in pain perception [39]. The aINS activation reflects changes in not only emotional experience but also interoception, i.e., awareness of one’s internal bodily states [40]. Over-prediction of an aversive bodily state may potentiate anxiety and avoidance behavior [41]. Therefore, the aINS activation reported here may reflect the emotional awareness of a negative experience (e.g., fear) and an increased interoceptive experience (i.e., pain) related to treatment. The dACC may play an additional role in both avoidance behavior and the decision-making process [22, 42, 43]. When facing a threatening object, the dACC is associated with the selection of an action in response to the stimuli. In contrast, the aINS is associated with the awareness of threat [42]. Therefore, our findings suggest that the individual difference in threat perception may play a key role in the development of dental avoidance.

Our findings echoed the previous reports about the neural mechanisms of a specific phobia. For example, dental phobia was associated with heightened activation in the dACC and aINS when the subjects were viewing images or videos depicting dental procedures [10, 13]. In patients with spider phobia, dACC activation was associated with the degree of the visual avoidance of the images of spiders [44]. Our findings further suggest that pain is the primary negative experience that dental patients would choose to avoid. The activation of both aINS and dACC is associated with the painful experience elicited by viewing images depicting injury and replaying one’s past experience of pain [15, 18]. Increased dACC activation was found when subjects escaped from painful stimuli, which was an imminent threat [23]. Notably, right aINS activation was found exclusively during the REXP task. The results echoed a recent finding that aINS activation was found when the participants recalled their pain experience [19].

Functional connectivity related to dental avoidance

Neuroimaging evidence has shown that changes in aINS-dACC connectivity are associated with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and panic disorder [26, 27]. These disorders are characterized by a heightened perceived threat in the patients. The intrinsic functional connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex and the aINS was positively correlated with the score of harm avoidance, which is a marker of trait anxiety [45]. In our study, however, we did not find a change in aINS-dACC connectivity, as shown in the previous studies. A possible explanation is that psychological stress is milder in the case of dental visits than is the stress related to PTSD or panic disorders. Therefore, the effect of plasticity is less significant than it is in patients with mental disorders.

Still, we found that aINS-OFC connectivity was associated with the individual difference in dental avoidance. Structurally, the aINS is predominantly connected to the OFC [46], and the projection from the insula to the OFC may convey somatosensory information [47]. Functionally, the OFC is a critical region related to anxiety personality [48], particularly for fear related to pain [49]. The OFC also subserves the emotion-laden process of decision-making [50]. The insula, as the core of the intrinsic salience network, may integrate the signal for a high-salience object from a threatening context (e.g., viewing images depicting painful procedures) [51]. Therefore, aINS-OFC connectivity may represent the integration between threat perception and decision-making with respect to dental visits.

Limitations of the study

The findings should be cautiously interpreted due to some limitations of the experimental design. First, we did not perform an in-scan trial-by-trial rating of the treatment-related experience for each experiment trial. We considered that the in-scan rating procedure may interfere with the re-experiencing task and that the additional rating procedure may prolong the total scanning time. The trade-off is that we were not able to trace dynamic changes in the emotional status. Second, we did not assess the degree of vividness or accuracy of the treatment-related experience. Memories of traumatic events, such as receiving a stressful dental procedure, may be vulnerable to false memories [52]. For example, the participants may misattribute a painful experience, which was actually elicited by a root canal treatment, to a third molar extraction. In the current study, we did not separately investigate each of the 12 dental procedures. Instead, we pooled the ratings from the 12 dental procedures and formed an overall score on fear and avoidance.

Conclusions

The results from our experiments revealed that the regional activation and connectional changes related to the aINS and dACC were associated with the individual differences in dental avoidance. The findings highlighted that prior dental experience as a predominant factor in shaping patients’ avoidance behavior and that the individual differences in threat perception may play a key role in the development of dental avoidance.

References

  1. Anderson KO, Masur FT 3rd. Psychological preparation for invasive medical and dental procedures. J Behav Med. 1983;6(1):1–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Armfield JM, Stewart JF, Spencer AJ. The vicious cycle of dental fear: exploring the interplay between oral health, service utilization and dental fear. BMC oral health. 2007;7:1.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Poi PJ, Chuah SY, Srinivas P, Liam CK. Common fears of patients undergoing bronchoscopy. Eur Resp J. 1998;11(5):1147–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Wilson JF, Moore RW, Randolph S, Hanson BJ. Behavioral preparation of patients for gastrointestinal endoscopy: information, relaxation, and coping style. J Human Stress. 1982;8(4):13–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Armfield JM. What goes around comes around: revisiting the hypothesized vicious cycle of dental fear and avoidance. Commun Dent Oral Epidemiol. 2013;41(3):279–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. De Jongh A, Schutjes M, Aartman IH. A test of Berggren’s model of dental fear and anxiety. Eur J Oral Sci. 2011;119(5):361–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. de Jongh A, Aartman IH, Brand N. Trauma-related phenomena in anxious dental patients. Commun Dent Oral Epidemiol. 2003;31(1):52–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. van Wijk AJ, Hoogstraten J. Experience with dental pain and fear of dental pain. J Dent Res. 2005;84(10):947–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Olsson A, Phelps EA. Social learning of fear. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(9):1095–102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hilbert K, Evens R, Maslowski NI, Wittchen HU, Lueken U. Fear processing in dental phobia during crossmodal symptom provocation: an fMRI study. BioMed Res Int. 2014;2014:196353.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Lueken U, Hoyer J, Siegert J, Gloster AT, Wittchen HU. Symptom provocation in dental anxiety using cross-phobic video stimulation. Eur J Oral Sci. 2011;119(1):61–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Lueken U, Kruschwitz JD, Muehlhan M, Siegert J, Hoyer J, Wittchen HU. How specific is specific phobia? Different neural response patterns in two subtypes of specific phobia. NeuroImage. 2011;56(1):363–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Scharmuller W, Ubel S, Leutgeb V, Schoengassner F, Wabnegger A, Schienle A. Do not think about pain: neural correlates of attention guiding during visual symptom provocation in dental phobia–an fMRI study. Brain Res. 2014;1566:69–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Schienle A, Wabnegger A, Schoengassner F, Scharmuller W. Neuronal correlates of three attentional strategies during affective picture processing: an fMRI study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14(4):1320–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Decety J, Grezes J. The power of simulation: imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Res. 2006;1079(1):4–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Jackson PL, Rainville P, Decety J. To what extent do we share the pain of others? Insight from the neural bases of pain empathy. Pain. 2006;125(1–2):5–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(1):42–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Ogino Y, Nemoto H, Inui K, Saito S, Kakigi R, Goto F. Inner experience of pain: imagination of pain while viewing images showing painful events forms subjective pain representation in human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(5):1139–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fairhurst M, Fairhurst K, Berna C, Tracey I. An fMRI study exploring the overlap and differences between neural representations of physical and recalled pain. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e48711.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Jackson PL, Brunet E, Meltzoff AN, Decety J. Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(5):752–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Bradley MM, Silakowski T, Lang PJ. Fear of pain and defensive activation. Pain. 2008;137(1):156–63.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. McNaughton N, Corr PJ. A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28(3):285–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Mobbs D, Marchant JL, Hassabis D, Seymour B, Tan G, Gray M, Petrovic P, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. From threat to fear: the neural organization of defensive fear systems in humans. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2009;29(39):12236–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Holtz K, Pane-Farre CA, Wendt J, Lotze M, Hamm AO. Brain activation during anticipation of interoceptive threat. NeuroImage. 2012;61(4):857–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:377–401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Pannekoek JN, Veer IM, van Tol MJ, van der Werff SJ, Demenescu LR, Aleman A, Veltman DJ, Zitman FG, Rombouts SA, van der Wee NJ. Aberrant limbic and salience network resting-state functional connectivity in panic disorder without comorbidity. J Affect Disord. 2013;145(1):29–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Tursich M, Ros T, Frewen PA, Kluetsch RC, Calhoun VD, Lanius RA. Distinct intrinsic network connectivity patterns of post-traumatic stress disorder symptom clusters. Acta psychiatrica Scandinavica 2015.

  28. Locker D, Shapiro D, Liddell A. Negative dental experiences and their relationship to dental anxiety. Community Dent Health. 1996;13(2):86–92.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Locker D, Thomson WM, Poulton R. Psychological disorder, conditioning experiences, and the onset of dental anxiety in early adulthood. J Dent Res. 2001;80(6):1588–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. van Wijk AJ, Hoogstraten J. The fear of dental pain questionnaire: construction and validity. Eur J Oral Sci. 2003;111(1):12–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Humphris GM, Morrison T, Lindsay SJ. The modified dental anxiety scale: validation and United Kingdom norms. Community Dent Health. 1995;12(3):143–50.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Yuan S, Freeman R, Lahti S, Lloyd-Williams F, Humphris G. Some psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Modified Dental Anxiety Scale with cross validation. Health Quality Life Outcomes. 2008;6:22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Lin CS, Hsieh JC, Yeh TC, Niddam DM. Predictability-mediated pain modulation in context of multiple cues: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2014;64C:85–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Ku HL, Lin CS, Chao HT, Tu PC, Li CT, Cheng CM, Su TP, Lee YC, Hsieh JC. Brain signature characterizing the body-brain-mind axis of transsexuals. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e70808.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Brodersen KH, Wiech K, Lomakina EI, Lin CS, Buhmann JM, Bingel U, Ploner M, Stephan KE, Tracey I. Decoding the perception of pain from fMRI using multivariate pattern analysis. NeuroImage. 2012;63(3):1162–70.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Lin CS, Ku HL, Chao HT, Tu PC, Li CT, Cheng CM, Su TP, Lee YC, Hsieh JC. Neural network of body representation differs between transsexuals and cissexuals. PLoS One. 2014;9(1):e85914.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Halonen H, Salo T, Hakko H, Rasanen P. The association between dental anxiety, general clinical anxiety and depression among Finnish university students. Oral Health Dental Manage. 2014;13(2):320–5.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Wiech K, Lin CS, Brodersen KH, Bingel U, Ploner M, Tracey I. Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain. J Neurosci off J Soc Neurosci. 2010;30(48):16324–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Zaki J, Davis JI, Ochsner KN. Overlapping activity in anterior insula during interoception and emotional experience. NeuroImage. 2012;62(1):493–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Paulus MP, Stein MB. An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(4):383–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5–6):655–67.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron. 2013;79(2):217–40.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Aue T, Hoeppli ME, Piguet C, Sterpenich V, Vuilleumier P. Visual avoidance in phobia: particularities in neural activity, autonomic responding, and cognitive risk evaluations. Front Human Neurosci. 2013;7:194.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Markett S, Weber B, Voigt G, Montag C, Felten A, Elger C, Reuter M. Intrinsic connectivity networks and personality: the temperament dimension harm avoidance moderates functional connectivity in the resting brain. Neuroscience. 2013;240:98–105.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Wiech K, Jbabdi S, Lin CS, Andersson J, Tracey I. Differential structural and resting state connectivity between insular subdivisions and other pain-related brain regions. Pain. 2014;155(10):2047–55.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. Insula of the old world monkey. III: efferent cortical output and comments on function. J Comp Neurol. 1982;212(1):38–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Montag C, Reuter M, Jurkiewicz M, Markett S, Panksepp J. Imaging the structure of the human anxious brain: a review of findings from neuroscientific personality psychology. Rev Neurosci. 2013;24(2):167–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Ochsner KN, Ludlow DH, Knierim K, Hanelin J, Ramachandran T, Glover GC, Mackey SC. Neural correlates of individual differences in pain-related fear and anxiety. Pain. 2006;120(1–2):69–77.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3):295–307.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Phelps EA, Lempert KM, Sokol-Hessner P. Emotion and decision making: multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2014;37:263–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Morgan CA, Southwick S. Perspective: I believe what I remember, but it may not be true. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;112:101–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

CSL conceived the study. CSL and LTW carried out the experiment and prepared study materials. CSL and SYW contributed to the analysis and interpretation of the data and prepared the manuscript. CSL drafted the manuscript. CSL, SYW and LT W discussed the framework and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

C-S. Lin was funded by National Science Council of Taiwan (NSC 102-2314-B-010-049). This work was supported in part by the 3T MRI Core Facility at National Yang-Ming University.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chia-Shu Lin.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lin, CS., Wu, SY. & Wu, LT. The anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortex are associated with avoidance of dental treatment based on prior experience of treatment in healthy adults. BMC Neurosci 16, 88 (2015). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12868-015-0224-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12868-015-0224-9

Keywords